Art Labeling Activity Figure 53 1 of 2 21 of 32

Foundations

  • Interactions with Adults
  • Relationships with Adults
  • Interactions with Peers
  • Relationships with Peers
  • Identity of Self in Relation to Others
  • Recognition of Ability
  • Expression of Emotion
  • Empathy
  • Emotion Regulation
  • Impulse Control
  • Social Agreement

References

Return to Contents

Social-emotional development includes the child's experience, expression, and direction of emotions and the ability to establish positive and rewarding relationships with others (Cohen and others 2005). It encompasses both intra- and interpersonal processes.

The cadre features of emotional development include the power to identify and sympathize one'due south ain feelings, to accurately read and cover emotional states in others, to manage strong emotions and their expression in a constructive manner, to regulate i's own behavior, to develop empathy for others, and to establish and maintain relationships. (National Scientific Council on the Developing Child 2004, 2)

Infants experience, express, and perceive emotions earlier they fully sympathize them. In learning to recognize, characterization, manage, and communicate their emotions and to perceive and attempt to sympathise the emotions of others, children build skills that connect them with family unit, peers, teachers, and the community. These growing capacities aid young children to go competent in negotiating increasingly complex social interactions, to participate effectively in relationships and group activities, and to reap the benefits of social support crucial to healthy human evolution and functioning.

Good for you social-emotional development for infants and toddlers unfolds in an interpersonal context, namely that of positive ongoing relationships with familiar, nurturing adults. Immature children are particularly attuned to social and emotional stimulation. Even newborns announced to nourish more than to stimuli that resemble faces (Johnson and others 1991). They likewise prefer their mothers' voices to the voices of other women (DeCasper and Fifer 1980). Through nurturance, adults support the infants' primeval experiences of emotion regulation (Bronson 2000a; Thompson and Goodvin 2005).

Responsive caregiving supports infants in beginning to regulate their emotions and to develop a sense of predictability, prophylactic, and responsiveness in their social environments. Early relationships are and so of import to developing infants that research experts accept broadly concluded that, in the early years, "nurturing, stable and consistent relationships are the key to healthy growth, development and learning" (National Research Council and Institute of Medicine 2000, 412). In other words, high-quality relationships increase the likelihood of positive outcomes for young children (Shonkoff 2004). Experiences with family unit members and teachers provide an opportunity for immature children to larn about social relationships and emotions through exploration and predictable interactions. Professionals working in child care settings can back up the social-emotional evolution of infants and toddlers in various means, including interacting directly with young children, communicating with families, arranging the physical space in the care environment, and planning and implementing curriculum.

Encephalon enquiry indicates that emotion and knowledge are greatly interrelated processes. Specifically, "recent cognitive neuroscience findings suggest that the neural mechanisms underlying emotion regulation may be the same every bit those underlying cognitive processes" (Bell and Wolfe 2004, 366). Emotion and cognition work together, jointly informing the kid'due south impressions of situations and influencing behavior. Virtually learning in the early years occurs in the context of emotional supports (National Research Council and Institute of Medicine 2000). "The rich interpenetrations of emotions and cognitions establish the major psychic scripts for each child'south life" (Panksepp 2001). Together, emotion and noesis contribute to attentional processes, determination making, and learning (Cacioppo and Berntson 1999). Furthermore, cognitive processes, such as decision making, are affected past emotion (Barrett and others 2007). Brain structures involved in the neural circuitry of cognition influence emotion and vice versa (Barrett and others 2007). Emotions and social behaviors impact the young child's ability to persist in goal-oriented activity, to seek help when it is needed, and to participate in and benefit from relationships.

Young children who exhibit healthy social, emotional, and behavioral adjustment are more probable to accept practiced bookish functioning in elementary schoolhouse (Cohen and others 2005; Zip to Three 2004). The sharp distinction between cognition and emotion that has historically been fabricated may exist more of an artifact of scholarship than information technology is representative of the manner these processes occur in the brain (Barrett and others 2007). This recent enquiry strengthens the view that early childhood programs support later positive learning outcomes in all domains by maintaining a focus on the promotion of healthy social emotional evolution (National Scientific Quango on the Developing Child 2004; Raver 2002; Shonkoff 2004).

Interactions with Adults

Interactions with adults are a frequent and regular part of infants' daily lives. Infants every bit young as three months of age have been shown to exist able to discriminate betwixt the faces of unfamiliar adults (Barrera and Maurer 1981). The foundations that draw Interactions with Adults and Relationships with Adults are interrelated. They jointly give a picture of healthy social-emotional development that is based in a supportive social environment established by adults. Children develop the ability to both answer to adults and engage with them first through predictable interactions in shut relationships with parents or other caring adults at habitation and outside the dwelling house. Children utilise and build upon the skills learned through close relationships to collaborate with less familiar adults in their lives. In interacting with adults, children appoint in a wide diversity of social exchanges such as establishing contact with a relative or engaging in storytelling with an infant care teacher.

Quality in early childhood programs is, in large part, a part of the interactions that accept identify between the adults and children in those programs. These interactions form the footing for the relationships that are established between teachers and children in the classroom or home and are related to children'south developmental condition. How teachers interact with children is at the very heart of early babyhood education (Kontos and Wilcox-Herzog 1997, 11).

Foundation: Interactions with Adults

Render to Top

Relationships with Adults

Close relationships with adults who provide consistent nurturance strengthen children's capacity to larn and develop. Moreover, relationships with parents, other family unit members, caregivers, and teachers provide the key context for infants' social-emotional development. These special relationships influence the infant's emerging sense of self and understanding of others. Infants use relationships with adults in many ways: for reassurance that they are safe, for help in alleviating distress, for help with emotion regulation, and for social approval or encouragement. Establishing close relationships with adults is related to children'south emotional security, sense of self, and evolving understanding of the earth around them. Concepts from the literature on attachment may exist applied to early on childhood settings, in considering the infant care teacher'southward function in separations and reunions during the twenty-four hour period in care, facilitating the child's exploration, providing condolement, meeting physical needs, modeling positive relationships, and providing support during stressful times (Raikes 1996).

Foundation: Relationships with Adults

Render to Meridian

Interactions with Peers

In early infancy children interact with each other using uncomplicated behaviors such as looking at or touching another child. Infants' social interactions with peers increase in complexity from engaging in repetitive or routine back-and-forth interactions with peers (for example, rolling a ball back and forth) to engaging in cooperative activities such as building a belfry of blocks together or acting out dissimilar roles during pretend play. Through interactions with peers, infants explore their interest in others and acquire about social beliefs/social interaction. Interactions with peers provide the context for social learning and problem solving, including the experience of social exchanges, cooperation, plough-taking, and the sit-in of the kickoff of empathy. Social interactions with peers also allow older infants to experiment with dissimilar roles in small-scale groups and in dissimilar situations such as relating to familiar versus unfamiliar children. As noted, the foundations called Interactions with Adults, Relationships with Adults, Interactions with Peers, and Relationships with Peers are interrelated. Interactions are stepping-stones to relationships. Burk (1996, 285) writes:

We, as teachers, need to facilitate the evolution of a psychologically condom environment that promotes positive social interaction. Equally children interact openly with their peers, they learn more about each other every bit individuals, and they begin building a history of interactions.

Foundation: Interaction with Peers

Return to Acme

Relationships with Peers

Infants develop close relationships with children they know over a period of time, such as other children in the family unit child care setting or neighborhood. Relationships with peers provide young children with the opportunity to develop strong social connections. Infants oftentimes prove a preference for playing and existence with friends, as compared with peers with whom they do non have a relationship. Howes' (1983) research suggests that there are distinctive patterns of friendship for the infant, toddler, and preschooler historic period groups. The 3 groups vary in the number of friendships, the stability of friendships, and the nature of interaction between friends (for example, the extent to which they involve object exchange or verbal communication).

Foundation: Relationships with Peers

Return to Top

Identity of Self in Relation to Others

Infants' social-emotional development includes an emerging awareness of self and others. Infants demonstrate this foundation in a number of ways. For case, they can respond to their names, point to their body parts when asked, or proper noun members of their families. Through an emerging understanding of other people in their social environs, children proceeds an understanding of their roles within their families and communities. They too go aware of their own preferences and characteristics and those of others.

Foundation: Identity of Self in Relation to Others

Return to Top

Recognition of Ability

Infants' developing sense of self-efficacy includes an emerging understanding that they can make things happen and that they accept particular abilities. Self-efficacy is related to a sense of competency, which has been identified as a basic human need (Connell 1990). The development of children'due south sense of cocky-efficacy may be seen in play or exploratory behaviors when they human action on an object to produce a result. For example, they pat a musical toy to brand sounds come out. Older infants may demonstrate recognition of power through "I" statements, such as "I did it" or "I'1000 good at drawing."

Foundation: Recognition of Ability

Return to Top

Expression of Emotion

Fifty-fifty early in infancy, children express their emotions through facial expressions, vocalizations, and body language. The later ability to apply words to express emotions gives immature children a valuable tool in gaining the assistance or social support of others (Saarni and others 2006). Temperament may play a office in children's expression of emotion. Tronick (1989, 112) described how expression of emotion is related to emotion regulation and communication between the mother and babe: "the emotional expressions of the babe and the caretaker part to permit them to mutually regulate their interactions . . . the babe and the adult are participants in an affective communication system."

Both the understanding and expression of emotion are influenced by culture. Cultural factors affect children's growing understanding of the meaning of emotions, the developing noesis of which situations lead to which emotional outcomes, and their learning virtually which emotions are appropriate to display in which situations (Thompson and Goodvin 2005). Some cultural groups appear to express certain emotions more oft than other cultural groups (Tsai, Levenson, and McCoy 2006). In addition, cultural groups vary by which particular emotions or emotional states they value (Tsai, Knutson, and Fung 2006). One study suggests that cultural differences in exposure to particular emotions through storybooks may contribute to immature children's preferences for detail emotional states (for example, excited or calm) (Tsai and others 2007).

Young children's expression of positive and negative emotions may play a significant role in their development of social relationships. Positive emotions appeal to social partners and seem to enable relationships to form, while problematic direction or expression of negative emotions leads to difficulty in social relationships (Denham and Weissberg 2004). The use of emotion-related words appears to be associated with how likable preschoolers are considered by their peers. Children who use emotion-related words were establish to be improve-liked by their classmates (Fabes and others 2001). Infants reply more positively to developed vocalizations that have a positive affective tone (Fernald 1993). Social smiling is a developmental process in which neurophysiology and cerebral, social, and emotional factors play a function, seen every bit a "reflection and constituent of an interactive relationship" (Messinger and Fogel 2007, 329). Information technology appears likely that the experience of positive emotions is a specially important contributor to emotional well-being and psychological health (Fredrickson 2000, 2003; Panksepp 2001).

Foundation: Expression of Emotion

Return to Top

Empathy

During the first three years of life, children begin to develop the capacity to experience the emotional or psychological country of another person (Zahn-Waxler and Radke-Yarrow 1990). The following definitions of empathy are establish in the research literature: "knowing what another person is feeling," "feeling what another person is feeling," and "responding compassionately to some other'south distress" (Levenson and Ruef 1992, 234). The concept of empathy reflects the social nature of emotion, equally it links the feelings of two or more people (Levenson and Ruef 1992). Since human life is relationship-based, one vitally important function of empathy over the life span is to strengthen social bonds (Anderson and Keltner 2002). Research has shown a correlation betwixt empathy and prosocial behavior (Eisenberg 2000). In particular, prosocial behaviors, such every bit helping, sharing, and comforting or showing business for others, illustrate the development of empathy (Zahn-Waxler and others 1992) and how the feel of empathy is idea to be related to the development of moral behavior (Eisenberg 2000). Adults model prosocial/empathic behaviors for infants in various ways. For example, those behaviors are modeled through caring interactions with others or through providing nurturance to the infant. Quann and Wien (2006, 28) suggest that one way to support the development of empathy in young children is to create a culture of caring in the early babyhood environment: "Helping children sympathise the feelings of others is an integral aspect of the curriculum of living together. The relationships among teachers, between children and teachers, and among children are fostered with warm and caring interactions."

Foundation: Empathy

Return to Tiptop

Emotion Regulation

The developing ability to regulate emotions has received increasing attention in the research literature (Eisenberg, Champion, and Ma 2004). Researchers have generated various definitions of emotion regulation, and debate continues every bit to the most useful and appropriate way to define this concept (Eisenberg and Spinrad 2004). As a construct, emotion regulation reflects the interrelationship of emotions, cognitions, and behaviors (Bell and Wolfe 2004). Young children'south increasing understanding and skill in the use of linguistic communication is of vital importance in their emotional development, opening new avenues for communicating well-nigh and regulating emotions (Campos, Frankel, and Camras 2004) and helping children to negotiate acceptable outcomes to emotionally charged situations in more effective ways. Emotion regulation is influenced past culture and the historical era in which a person lives: cultural variability in regulation processes is meaning (Mesquita and Frijda 1992). "Cultures vary in terms of what 1 is expected to feel, and when, where, and with whom 1 may limited different feelings" (Cheah and Rubin 2003, 3). Adults can provide positive part models of emotion regulation through their behavior and through the verbal and emotional support they offering children in managing their emotions. Responsiveness to infants' signals contributes to the development of emotion regulation. Adults support infants' development of emotion regulation by minimizing exposure to excessive stress, chaotic environments, or over- or understimulation.

Emotion regulation skills are important in part because they play a part in how well children are liked by peers and teachers and how socially competent they are perceived to exist (National Scientific Council on the Developing Child 2004). Children's ability to regulate their emotions appropriately can contribute to perceptions of their overall social skills as well as to the extent to which they are liked by peers (Eisenberg and others 1993). Poor emotion regulation tin impair children's thinking, thereby compromising their judgment and decision making (National Scientific Quango on the Developing Child 2004). At kindergarten entry, children demonstrate wide variability in their ability to cocky-regulate (National Inquiry Council and Institute of Medicine 2000).

Foundation: Emotion Regulation

Return to Top

Impulse Command

Children'south developing capacity to control impulses helps them suit to social situations and follow rules. As infants grow, they become increasingly able to practice voluntary command over behavior such equally waiting for needs to exist met, inhibiting potentially hurtful behavior, and acting according to social expectations, including safety rules. Grouping intendance settings provide many opportunities for children to practice their impulse-control skills. Peer interactions oftentimes offer natural opportunities for young children to practice impulse control, as they brand progress in learning about cooperative play and sharing. Young children's understanding or lack of agreement of requests made of them may be one factor contributing to their responses (Kaler and Kopp 1990).

Foundation: Impulse Command

Return to Summit

Social Agreement

During the infant/toddler years, children begin to develop an agreement of the responses, communication, emotional expression, and deportment of other people. This development includes infants' understanding of what to expect from others, how to engage in back-and-forth social interactions, and which social scripts are to be used for which social situations. "At each age, social cognitive understanding contributes to social competence, interpersonal sensitivity, and an sensation of how the self relates to other individuals and groups in a complex social world" (Thompson 2006, 26). Social understanding is particularly important because of the social nature of humans and man life, even in early infancy (Wellman and Lagattuta 2000). Recent research suggests that infants' and toddlers' social understanding is related to how oftentimes they experience adult communication about the thoughts and emotions of others (Taumoepeau and Ruffman 2008).

Foundation: Social Understanding

Render to Top

References

Ainsworth, 1000. D. 1967. Infancy in Uganda: Infant Care and the Growth of Honey. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

American Academy of Pediatrics. 2004. Caring for Your Baby and Young Child: Birth to Historic period 5 (Quaternary edition). Edited by S. P. Shelov and R. Due east. Hannemann. New York: Bantam Books.

Anderson, C., and D. Keltner. 2002. "The Part of Empathy in the Germination and Maintenance of Social Bonds," Behavioral and Brain Sciences, Vol. 25, No. 1, 21–22.

Barrera, M. East., and D. Maurer. 1981. "Discrimination of Strangers by the Three-Month-Sometime," Child Development, Vol. 52, No. 2, 558–63.

Barrett, L., and others. 2007. "The Experience of Emotion," Annual Review of Psychology, Vol.58, 373–403.

Bates, E. 1990. "Language Nigh Me and You: Pronomial Reference and the Emerging Concept of Self," in The Self in Transition: Infancy to Childhood. Edited past D. Cicchetti and M. Beeghly. Chicago: Academy of Chicago Press.

Bayley, N. 2006. Bayley Scales of Baby and Toddler Development (Third edition). San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment, Inc.

Bell, Grand., and C. Wolfe. 2004. "Emotion and Noesis: An Intricately Bound Developmental Procedure," Kid Development, Vol. 75, No. ii, 366–70.

Bergman, A., and A. Wilson. 1984. "Thoughts Almost Stages on the Way to Empathy and the Capacity for Concern," in Empathy Two. Edited past J. Lichtenberg, M. Bornstein, and D. Silver. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Bowlby, J. 1983. Zipper (2nd edition), Attachment and Loss series, Vol. 1. Foreword by Allan Northward. Schore. New York: Basic Books.

Brazelton, T. B. 1992. Touchpoints: Your Child's Emotional and Behavioral Development. New York: Perseus Publishing.

Bretherton, I., and others. 1986. "Learning to Talk About Emotions: A Functionalist Perspective," Child Development, Vol. 57, 529–48.

Bridges, L. J.; West. South. Grolnick; and J. P Connell. 1997. "Baby Emotion Regulation with Mothers and Fathers," Babe Behavior and Evolution, Vol. 20, 47–57.

Bronson, Thousand. Due west. 1972. "Infants' Reaction to Unfamiliar Persons and Novel Objects," Monographs of the Club for Research in Kid Development, Vol. 37, No. 148, i–46.

Bronson, M. 2000a. "Recognizing and Supporting the Development of Self-Regulation in Young Children," Young Children, Vol. 55, No. 2, 32–37.

Bronson, M. 2000b. Self-Regulation in Early Childhood: Nature and Nurture. New York: Guilford Printing.

Bullock, M., and P. Lutkenhaus. 1988. "The Evolution of Volitional Behavior in the Toddler Years," Kid Development, Vol. 59, 664–74.

Bullock, M., and P. Lutkenhaus. 1990. "Who Am I? Self-Understanding in Toddlers," Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, Vol. 36, 217–38.

Burk, D. I. 1996. "Agreement Friendship and Social Interaction," Childhood Education, Vol. 72, No. 5, 282–85.

Cacioppo, J., and G. Berntson. 1999. "The Bear upon System: Architecture and Operating Characteristics," Current Directions in Psychological Science, Vol. 8, No. 5, 133–37.

California Section of Pedagogy (CDE). 2005. "Desired Results Developmental Profile (DRDP)" (accessed February vii, 2007) Sacramento: California Department of Pedagogy.

Return to Acme

Campos, J.; C. Frankel; and L. Camras. 2004. "On the Nature of Emotion Regulation," Child Development, Vol. 75, No. 2, 377–94.

Caufield, R. 1995. "Reciprocity Between Infants and Caregivers During the Showtime Yr of Life," Early on Babyhood Education Journal, Vol. 23, No. 1, three–8.

Cheah, C., and K. Rubin. 2003. "European American and Mainland Chinese Mothers' Socialization Beliefs Regarding Preschoolers' Social Skills," Parenting: Science and Practice, Vol. three, No. 1, one–21

Cohen, J., and others. 2005. Helping Immature Children Succeed: Strategies to Promote Early Childhood Social and Emotional Development. Washington, DC: National Conference of State Legislatures and Nothing to 3. (accessed on December seven, 2006)

Connell, J. P. 1990. "Context, Self, and Action: A Motivational Assay of Self-Organisation Processes Across the Lifespan," in The Self in Transition: Infancy to Childhood. Edited by D. Cicchetti and M. Beeghly. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.

Coplan, J. 1993. Early Language Milestone Scale: Examiner's Manual (2d edition). Austin, TX: Pro-ed.

Davies, D. 2004. Child Evolution: A Practitioner'southward Guide (Second edition). New York: Guilford Press.

DeCasper, A. J., and W. P. Fifer. 1980. "Of Human Bonding: Newborns Prefer Their Mothers' Voices," Science, Vol. 208, No. 6, 1174–76.

Denham, S. 1998. Emotional Development in Immature Children. New York: Guilford Press.

Denham, South., and R. Weissberg. 2003. "Social-Emotional Learning in Early on Childhood: What We Know and Where to Go From Hither," in A Pattern for the Promotion of Prosocial Behavior in Early on Childhood. Edited by E. Chesebrough and others. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.

Dickstein, S., and R. D. Parke. 1988. "Social Referencing in Infancy: A Glance at Fathers and Matrimony," Child Evolution, Vol. 59, No. ii, 506–11.

Dunn, J. 1983. "Sibling Relationships in Early Childhood," Child Development, Vol. 54, No. 4, 787–811.

Dunn, J. 1987. "The Ancestry of Moral Agreement: Development in the 2nd Year," in The Emergence of Morality in Young Children. Edited by J. Kagan and S. Lamb. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Dunn, J. 1988. The Beginnings of Social Agreement. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Printing.

Dunn, J. 1994. "Changing Minds and Changing Relationships," in Children's Early Understanding of Listen: Origins and Development. Edited by C. Lewis and P. Mitchell. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Dunn, J.; I. Bretherton; and P. Munn. 1987. "Conversations About Feeling States Between Mothers and Their Young Children," Developmental Psychology, Vol. 23, No. i, 132–39.

Eisenberg, Due north. 2000. "Emotion, Regulation and Moral Development," Annual Review of Psychology, Vol. 51, 665–97.

Eisenberg, North.; C. Champion; and Y. Ma. 2004. "Emotion-Related Regulation: An Emerging Construct," Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, Vol. l, No. 3, 236–59.

Eisenberg, Due north., and others. 1993. "The Relations of Emotionality and Regulation to Preschoolers' Social Skills and Sociometric Condition," Child Development, Vol. 64, 1418–38.

Eisenberg, Due north., and T. Spinrad. 2004. "Emotion-Related Regulation: Sharpening the Definition," Child Development, Vol. 75, No. 2, 334–39.

Return to Peak

Fabes, R., and others. 2001. "Preschoolers' Spontaneous Emotion Vocabulary: Relations to Likability," Early Education & Evolution, Vol. 12, No. 1, 11–27.

Fernald, A. 1993. "Approval and Disapproval: Infant Responsiveness to Song Impact in Familiar and Unfamiliar Languages," Child Development, Vol. 64, No. 3, 657–74.

Fogel, A. 2001. Infancy: Infant, Family, and Society (Fourth edition). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth/Thomson Learning.

Fox, N. A., and Southward. D. Calkins. 2000. "Multiple Measure Approaches to the Written report of Babe Emotion," in Handbook of Emotions (Second edition). Edited by M. Lewis and J. G. Haviland-Jones. New York: Guilford Press.

Fox, N. A., and R. J. Davidson. 1988. "Patterns of Brain Electrical Action During the Expression of Discrete Emotions in Ten-Month-Erstwhile Infants," Developmental Psychology, Vol.24, 230–36.

Frankenburg, W. One thousand., and others. 1990. Denver Ii Screening Manual. Denver, CO: Denver Developmental Materials.

Fredrickson, B. 2000. "Cultivating Positive Emotions to Optimize Wellness and Well-Being," Prevention and Handling, Vol. 3, No. 1.

Fredrickson, B. 2003. "The Value of Positive Emotions," American Scientist, Vol. 91, 330–35.

Greenspan, S., and Due north. T. Greenspan. 1985. Start Feelings: Milestones in the Emotional Development of Your Baby and Child. New York: Penguin Books.

Gustafson, Chiliad. Due east.; J. A. Green; and M. J. West. 1979. "The Infant's Irresolute Role in Female parent-Infant Games: The Growth of Social Skills," Infant Beliefs and Development, Vol. 2, 301–viii.

Harris, P. Fifty. 2000. "Understanding Emotion," in Handbook of Emotions (2d edition). Edited by Thousand. Lewis and J. 1000. Haviland-Jones. New York: Guilford Press.

Harris, P. Fifty., and others. 1989. "Young Children's Theory of Listen and Emotion," Cognition and Emotion, Vol. 3, No. 4, 379–400.

Hart, B., and T. R. Risley. 1999. The Social World of Children: Learning to Talk. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing.

Hart, S., and others. 1998. "Jealousy Protests in Infants of Depressed Mothers," Baby Behavior and Evolution, Vol. 21, No. ane, 137–48.

Hay, D. F.; J. Pederson; and A. Nash. 1982. "Dyadic Interaction in the First Twelvemonth of Life," in Peer Relationships and Social Skills in Childhood. Edited by K. H. Rubin and H. S. Ross. New York: Springer-Verlag.

Hirshberg, L. Thou., and M. Svejda. 1990. "When Infants Await to Their Parents: Infants' Social Referencing of Mothers Compared to Fathers," Child Evolution, Vol. 61, No. 4, 1175–86.

Hoffman, M. L. 1982. "Development of Prosocial Motivation: Empathy and Guilt," in The Development of Prosocial Behavior. Edited by Due north. Eisenberg. New York: Academic Printing.

Howes, C. 1983. "Patterns of Friendship," Child Evolution, Vol. 54, No. 4, 1041–53.

Howes, C. 1987. "Social Competence with Peers in Young Children: Developmental Sequences," Developmental Review, Vol. vii, 252–72.

Howes, C. 1988. "Peer Interaction of Immature Children," Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, Vol. 53, No. 1.

Howes, C., and C. C. Matheson. 1992. "Sequences in the Development of Competent Play with Peers: Social and Social Pretend Play," Developmental Psychology, Vol. 28, No. 5, 961–74.

Johnson, Thou., and others. 1991. "Newborns' Preferential Tracking of Face-Like Stimuli and Its Subsequent Decline," Cognition, Vol. 40, Nos. 1-two, 1–19.

Johnstone, T., and K. R., Scherer. 2000. "Vocal Communication of Emotion," in Handbook of Emotions (2d edition). Edited past Yard. Lewis and J. Haviland-Jones. New York: Guilford Press.

Kaler, Southward. R., and C. B. Kopp. 1990. "Compliance and Comprehension in Very Immature Toddlers," Child Development, Vol. 61, No. vi, 1997–2003.

Kaye, One thousand., and A. Fogel. 1980. "The Temporal Construction of Face-to-Confront Advice Between Mothers and Infants," Developmental Psychology, Vol. 16, 454–64.

Kontos, South., and A. Wilcox-Herzog. Jan, 1997. "Inquiry in Review: Teacher's Interactions with Children: Why Are They And so Important?" Young Children, Vol. 52, No. two, 4–12.

Kopp, C. 1989. "Regulation of Distress and Negative Emotions: A Developmental View," Developmental Psychology, Vol. 25, No. three, 343–54.

Return to Tiptop

Kravitz, H.; D. Goldenberg; and A. Neyhus. 1978. "Tactual Exploration by Normal Infants," Developmental Medicine and Child-Neurology, Vol. 20, No. 6, 720–26.

Lagattuta, 1000. H., and R. A. Thompson. 2007. "The Development of Self-Witting Emotions: Cognitive Processes and Social Influences," in The Self-Conscious Emotions: Theory and Research. Edited by J. L. Tracy, R. W. Robins, and J. P. Tangney. New York: Guilford Press.

Lally, J. R., and others. 1995. Caring for Infants and Toddlers in Groups: Developmentally Appropriate Practice. Washington, DC: Zero to 3 Press.

Lamb, Thousand. E.; 1000. H. Bornstein; and D. M. Teti. 2002. Development in Infancy: An Introduction (Fourth edition). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Lerner, C., and 50. A. Ciervo. 2003. Salubrious Minds: Nurturing Children's Development from 0 to 36 Months. Washington, DC: Zero to Three Press and American University of Pediatrics.

Lerner, C., and A. Fifty. Dombro. 2000. Learning and Growing Together: Understanding and Supporting Your Kid'due south Development. Washington, DC: Zero to Three Press.

Levenson, R., and A. Ruef. 1992. "Empathy: A Physiological Substrate," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 63, No. ii, 234–46.

Levine, L. Due east. 1983. "Mine: Cocky-Definition in two-Year-Former Boys," Developmental Psychology, Vol. 19, 544–49.

Lewis, M. 2000a. "The Emergence of Human Emotions," in Handbook of Emotions (Second edition). Edited past Thousand. Lewis and J. M. Haviland-Jones. New York: Guilford Printing.

Lewis, M. 2000b. "Cocky-Witting Emotions: Embarrassment, Pride, Shame, and Guilt," in Handbook of Emotions (Second edition). Edited by M. Lewis and J. M. Haviland-Jones. New York: Guilford Printing.

Lewis, G., and J. Brooks-Gunn. 1979. Social Cognition and the Acquisition of Cocky. New York: Plenum Press.

Lewis, M., and others. 1989. "Cocky Development and Self-Conscious Emotions," Kid Evolution, Vol. lx, No. 1, 146–56.

Lieberman, A. F. 1993. The Emotional Life of the Toddler. New York: Free Press.

Marvin, R., and P. Britner. 1999. "Normative Evolution: The Ontogeny of Zipper," in Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Enquiry, and Clinical Applications. Edited by J. Cassidy and P. Shaver. New York: Guilford Press.

McCarty, M. Eastward.; R. K. Clifton; and R. R. Collard. 1999. "Problem Solving in Infancy: The Emergence of an Action Plan," Developmental Psychology, Vol. 35, No. iv, 1091–1101.

Meisels, Southward. J., and others. 2003. The Ounce Calibration: Standards for the Developmental Profiles (Birth–42 Months). New York: Pearson Early on Learning.

Melson, G., and A. Cohen. 1981. "Contextual Influences on Children's Activity: Sexual practice Differences in Effects of Peer Presence and Interpersonal Attraction," Genetic Psychology Monographs, Vol. 103, 243–60.

Messinger, D., and A. Fogel. 2007. "The Interactive Development of Social Smiling," in Advances in Child Development and Beliefs (Vol. 35). Edited by R. V. Kail. Burlington, MA: Elsevier.

Mesquita, B., and Due north. Frijda. 1992. "Cultural Variations in Emotions: A Review," Psychological Bulletin, Vol. 112, No. 2, 179–204.

Mueller, East., and T. Lucas. 1975. "A Developmental Analysis of Peer Interaction Among Toddlers," in Friendship and Peer Relations. Edited past Yard. Lewis and 50. Rosenblum. New York: Wiley.

National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. 2000. From Neurons to Neighborhoods: The Scientific discipline of Early Childhood Development. Committee on Integrating the Science of Early Childhood Development. Edited by J. P. Shonkoff and D. A. Phillips. Board on Children, Youth and Families, Commission on Behavioral and Social Sciences and Pedagogy. Washington, DC: National Academies Press.

National Scientific Council on the Developing Child. Winter, 2004. "Children'due south Emotional Development Is Congenital into the Architecture of Their Brains" Working Paper No. ii External link opens in new window or tab.. (accessed on December 5, 2006)

Return to Tiptop

 Panksepp, J. 2001. The Long-Term Psychobiological Consequences of Infant Emotions: Prescriptions for the Twenty-First Century," Infant Mental Health Journal, Vol. 22, No. 1–2, 132–73.

Parks, Due south. 2004. Within HELP: Hawaii Early Learning Profile Assistants and Reference Manual. Palo Alto, CA: VORT Corporation.

Parritz, R. H. 1996. "A Descriptive Analysis of Toddler Coping in Challenging Circumstances," Infant Beliefs and Development, Vol. 19, 171–80.

Pruett, K. D. 1999. Me, Myself and I: How Children Build Their Sense of Self (eighteen to 36 Months). New York: Goddard Press.

Quann, Five., and C. Wien. 2006. "The Visible Empathy of Infants and Toddlers," Immature Children, Vol. 61, No. iv, 22–29.

Raikes, H. 1996. "A Secure Base for Babies: Applying Attachment Concepts to the Infant Care Setting," Young Children, Vol. 51, No. 5, 59–67.

Raver, C. 2002. "Emotions Matter: Making the Case for the Role of Immature Children'south Emotional Evolution for Early School Readiness," SRCD Social Policy Report, Vol. 16, No. 3.

Ross, H. S., and B. D. Goldman. 1977. "Establishing New Social Relations in Infancy," in Attachment Behavior. Edited by T. Alloway, P. Pliner, and L. Krames. New York: Plenum Press.

Rothbart, G. One thousand.; H. Ziaie; and C. G. O'Boyle. 1992. "Self-Regulation and Emotion in Infancy," in Emotion and Its Regulation in Early Development (No. 55). Edited by N. Eisenberg and R. Fabes. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass/Pfeiffer.

Ryalls, B.; R. Gul; and Chiliad. Ryalls. 2000. "Baby Imitation of Peer and Adult Models: Bear witness for a Peer Model Reward," Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, Vol. 46, No.1, 188–202.

Saarni, C., and others. 2006. "Emotional Evolution: Action, Advice, and Understanding," in Handbook of Child Psychology (6th edition), Vol. 3, Social, Emotional, and Personality Development. Edited by N. Eisenberg. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley and Sons.

Schaffer, H. R., and P. E. Emerson. 1964. "The Development of Social Attachments in Infancy," Monographs of the Guild for Research in Kid Evolution, Vol. 29, No. 3.

Segal, M. 2004. "The Roots and Fruits of Pretending," in Children's Play: The Roots of Reading. Edited by East. F. Zigler, D. G. Singer, and S. J. Bishop-Josef. Washington, DC: Zero to Iii Printing.

Shonkoff, J. P. 2004. Science, Policy and the Developing Kid: Closing the Gap Betwixt What We Know and What We Exercise. Washington, DC: Ounce of Prevention Fund. http://www.ounceofprevention.org/downloads/publications/shonkoffweb.pdf (accessed on December vii, 2006) Annotation, the preceding Web address is no longer valid.

Siegel, D. J. 1999. The Developing Mind: How Relationships and the Encephalon Interact to Shape Who We Are. New York: Guilford Press.

Squires, J.; D. Bricker; and E. Twombly. 2002. The Ages & Stages Questionnaires: Social-Emotional. A Parent-Completed Child-Monitoring Arrangement for Social-Emotional Behaviors ASQ:SE. User's Guide. Baltimore, Medico: Paul H. Brookes Publishing.

Sroufe, 50. A. 1979. "Socioemotional Development," in Handbook of Infant Development. Edited past J. Osofsky. New York: Wiley.

Stern, D. Due north. 1985. The Interpersonal Earth of the Infant: A View from Psychoanalysis and Developmental Psychology. New York: Basic Books.

Sternberg, C. R.; J. J. Campos; and R. N. Emde. 1983. "The Facial Expression of Anger in Seven-Month-Old Infants," Kid Development, Vol. 54, 178–84.

Stipek, D. J.; J. H. Gralinski; and C. B. Kopp. 1990. "Self-Concept Evolution in the Toddler Years," Developmental Psychology, Vol. 26, No. 6, 972–77.

Taumoepeau, M., and T. Ruffman. 2008. "Stepping Stones to Others' Minds: Maternal Talk Relates to Child Mental State Language and Emotion Understanding at 15, 24, and 33 Months," Child Development, Vol. 79, No. two, 284–302.

Teti, D. M. 1999. "Conceptualizations of Disorganization in the Preschool Years: An Integration," in Attachment Disorganization. Edited past J. Solomon and C. George. New York: Guilford Press.

Render to Top

Thelen, E., and A. Fogel. 1989. "Toward an Action-Based Theory of Babe Evolution," in Activeness in Social Context: Perspectives on Early Development. Edited past J. Lockman and Due north. Hazen. New York: Plenum Press.

Thompson, R. A. 1987. "Empathy and Emotional Understanding: The Early Evolution of Empathy," in Empathy and Its Evolution. Edited by N. Eisenberg and J. Strayer. New York: Cambridge Academy Press.

Thompson, R. A. 2006. "The Development of the Person: Social Understanding, Relationships, Self, Conscience," in Handbook of Child Psychology (Sixth edition), Volume 3: Social, Emotional, and Personality Development. Edited by N. Eisenberg. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley and Sons.

Thompson, R. A., and R. Goodvin. 2005. "The Individual Child: Temperament, Emotion, Self and Personality," in Developmental Science: An Advanced Textbook (Fifth edition). Edited past M. H. Bornstein and M. E. Lamb. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Tronick, Eastward. Z. 1989. "Emotions and Emotional Communication in Infants," American Psychologist, Vol. 44, No. ii, 112–19.

Tsai, J.; B. Knutson; and H. Fung. 2006. "Cultural Variation in Bear upon Valuation," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 90, No. ii, 288–307.

Tsai, J.; R. Levenson; and K. McCoy. 2006. "Cultural and Temperamental Variation in Emotional Response," Emotion, Vol. vi, No. 3, 484–97.

Tsai, J., and others. 2007. "Learning What Feelings to Want: Socialization of Platonic Affect Through Children's Storybooks," Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Vol. 33, No. i, 17–30.

Weinberg, M. K., and others. 1999. "Gender Differences in Emotional Expressivity and Cocky-Regulation During Early Infancy," Developmental Psychology, Vol. 35, No. 1, 175–88.

Wellman, H. Thou., and G. H. Lagattuta. 2000. "Developing Understandings of Mind," in Agreement Other Minds: Perspectives from Developmental Cerebral Neuroscience. Edited by S. Baron-Cohen, T. Tager-Flusberg, and D. J. Cohen. New York: Oxford University Printing.

Wingert, P., and G. Brant. 2005. "Reading Your Baby'south Heed," Newsweek, Aug. fifteen, 2005, 32–39.

Yuill, Northward. 1984. "Young Children'southward Coordination of Motive and Upshot in Judgments of Satisfaction and Morality," British Journal of Developmental Psychology, Vol. ii, 73–81.

Zahn-Waxler, C., and M. Radke-Yarrow. 1982. "The Development of Altruism: Alternative Enquiry Strategies," in The Development of Prosocial Behavior. Edited past N. Eisenberg. New York: Academic Press.

Zahn-Waxler, C., and Thousand. Radke-Yarrow. 1990. "The Origins of Empathetic Concern," Motivation and Emotion, Vol. 14, 107–30.

Zahn-Waxler, C., and others. 1992. "Development of Concern for Others," Developmental Psychology, Vol. 28, No. 1, 126–36.

Zahn-Waxler, C.; J. Robinson; and R. Emde. 1992. "Development of Empathy in Twins," Developmental Psychology, Vol. 28, No. six, 1038–47.

Zip to Three. 2004. Infant and Early Childhood Mental Health: Promoting Healthy Social and Emotional Development. Fact Sheet. May 18, 2004. Washington, DC: Zilch to Three. (accessed on December 7, 2006)

Return to Peak

Return to Contents

Last Reviewed: Thursday, June 17, 2021

goodmanament1937.blogspot.com

Source: https://www.cde.ca.gov/sp/cd/re/itf09socemodev.asp

0 Response to "Art Labeling Activity Figure 53 1 of 2 21 of 32"

Post a Comment

Iklan Atas Artikel

Iklan Tengah Artikel 1

Iklan Tengah Artikel 2

Iklan Bawah Artikel